#ASTDTK2013 Resources from ASTD Presentations

ASTD TK 2013 has been a great time. I’ve enjoyed spending time with old friends and making new ones

Here are the slides from my presentation, my workshop on “Games and Gamification” with Koreen Olbrish will be in another post.

Here are some related resources that you may find of value.

1. An experience as an avatar can change a person’s real life perceptions. In a study conducted by Yee and Bailenson (2007), it was found that negative stereotyping of the elderly was significantly reduced when participants were placed in avatars of old people compared with those participants placed in avatars of young people

Reference: Yee, N., & Bailenson, J.N. (2006). Walk a mile in digital shoes: The impact of embodied perspective-taking on the reduction of negative stereotyping in immersive virtual environments. Proceedings of PRESENCE 2006: The 9th Annual International Workshop on Presence. August 24- 26, Cleveland, Ohio, USA. See more research at the Stanford Virtual Human Interaction Lab.

2. It is better to have one “expert” avatar and another “motivational” avatar in a learning environment rather than having one combined “mentor” avatar. Baylor and Kim (2005) report that in multiple studies with avatars of different gender and race, evidence indicates that students learned significantly more and had significantly greater motivation when working with one motivator and a different expert avatar as compared to working with the just the one mentor avatar. This can be explained by the fact that it is easier for students to figuratively ‘compartmentalize’ the information from the avatar when it was delivered by two distinct sources.

Reference: Baylor, A. L. & Kim, Y. (2005). Simulating instructional roles through pedagogical agents. International Journal of Artificial Intelligence in Education, 15(1), 95-115.

Simulating Instructional Roles through Pedagogical Agents

Pedagogical Agents as Learning Companions: The Role of Agent Competency and Type
of Interaction.

3. Watching an avatar that looks like you performing an activity influences you to perform a similar or same activity in the future. Creating avatars and having a learner perform a task as an avatar can influence a person’s actual behavior outside of being an avatar. In one study, users watched an avatar that looked like them exercising and losing weight in a virtual environment, the result was that those that watched the avatar of themselves subsequently exercised more and ate healthier in the real world as compared to a control group. This as reported by Fox and Bailenson (2009). In similar study conducted by Yee, Bailenson & Ducheneaut, (2009), had three control group. One where participants were exposed to an avatar representing themselves running on a treadmill, the second with avatar running that did not represent the participant and the third group with avatar representing themselves loitering. Within 24 hours, after the experiment, participants who were exposed to the avatar running that represented themselves exercised significantly more than those in the other conditions. A study by Ersner-Hershfield et al. (2008) found that when college-aged students observed their avatar ageing in a virtual mirror, they formed a psychological connection to their “future self” and decided to invest more money in a retirement account as opposed to a control group.

Reference: Fox, J. & Bailenson, J. N. (2009).Virtual self-modeling: the effects of vicarious reinforcement and identification on exercise behaviors. Media Psychology. 12, 1–25. Ersner-Hershfield, H., Bailenson, J. & Carstensen, L. L. (2008). A vivid future self: immersive virtual reality enhances retirement saving. Chicago, IL: Association for Psychological Science. Yee, N., Bailenson, J.N., & Ducheneaut, N. (2009). The Proteus Effect: Implications of transformed digital self-representation on online and offline behavior. Communication Research, 36 (2), 285-312.

4. Trainees’ gain higher confidence in applying learning from a training session to their job when the training is simulation game based. The research evidence suggests the use of simulations to enhance the confidence trainees have in their ability to apply the skills learned in the training to their job. In a meta-analysis of more than 60 studies with 6,476 participants, it was found that trainees receiving instruction via a simulation game had 20% higher confidence they had learned the information taught in training and could perform the training-related tasks (self-efficacy) than trainees in a comparison group of more traditional methods.

Reference: Sitzmann, T. (2011) A meta-analytic examination of the instructional effectiveness of computer-based simulation games. Personnel Psychology.

5.Simulations embedded in a program of instruction are better tools for learning than stand alone simulations. Trainees learn more from simulations games that are embedded in a program of instruction than when simulation games are the sole instructional method. When simulation games were used as a supplement to other instructional methods, the simulation game group had higher knowledge levels than the comparison group. However, when simulation games were used as standalone instruction, trainees in a comparison group learned more than trainees in the simulation game group.

Reference: Sitzmann, T. (2011) A meta-analytic examination of the instructional effectiveness of computer-based simulation games. Personnel Psychology.

6.Simulation games don’t have to be entertaining to be educational. In a meta-analysis of studies, the research indicated that trainees learned the same amount of information in simulation games whether the games were ranked high in entertainment value or low in entertainment value. There does not appear to be a correlation between the entertainment value of a simulation game and its educational merit. (5 stars)

Reference: Sitzmann, T. (2011) A meta-analytic examination of the instructional effectiveness of computer-based simulation games. Personnel Psychology.

7. Trainees learn more from simulations games that actively engage trainees in learning rather than passively conveying the instructional material. When the majority of the instruction in a simulation game was passive, the comparison group learned more than the simulation game group. However, when the majority of the instruction in the simulation game was active, the simulation game group learned more than the comparison group. These findings suggest that simulation games are more effective when they actively engage trainees in learning the course material.

Reference: Sitzmann, T. (2011) A meta-analytic examination of the instructional effectiveness of computer-based simulation games. Personnel Psychology.

8.Trainees participating in simulation game learning experiences have higher declarative knowledge, procedural knowledge and retention of training material than those trainees participating in more traditional learning experiences. Post-training declarative knowledge, post-training procedural knowledge and retention of the training material is higher for trainees participating in a simulation game experience than the comparison groups. In examining the effectiveness of computer-based simulation games related to comparison groups, it was found that declarative knowledge was 11% higher for trainees taught with simulation games than a comparison group; procedural knowledge was 14% higher and retention was 9% higher.

Reference: Sitzmann, T. (2011) A meta-analytic examination of the instructional effectiveness of computer-based simulation games. Personnel Psychology.

9.The ability of simulations to teach skills that transfer to real-life, on-the-job situations seems abundantly positive, from the existing body of literature. Computer-based simulations—assessed as an alternative to other means of training, as a supplement to other means of training, as a device to combat skill decay in experienced trainees, and as a means of improving performance levels as they stand prior to training—show positive results for transfer a majority of the time: in 22 out of 26 such studies, trainees demonstrated equal or superior transfer to the control group from simulations.

Reference: Hahn, S. H. (2010). Transfer of training from simulations in civilian and military workforces: Perspectives from the current body of literature.

Blog posts discussing subjects covered in presentation:

Summary of #Games Research Qualitative Literature Review Conducted by Fengfeng Ke

Summary of a Literature Review on The Effectiveness of Instructional Games by Robert Hays
Teaching Games vs. Teaching Games

Using Fantasy in Instructional #Games

Consider Player/Game Interactions

Are you challenging your learners? Don’t treat them like babies.

Game Levels and Scaffolding–They’re Related

You want to teach leaders about Strategy: Have them Play Civilization V

In games and learning interfaces, strive for an interactive conversation interface

Combining Zombies and Running…Gamification of Exercise

Improve Training: Thinking Like a Game Developer

Accidental Learning and the Power of Stories

Take a Look: My Book on Gamification of Learning and Instruction

Articles
Fall 2011 Training Industry Quarterly Article: Improving Training: Thinking Like a Game Developer

July 2012 T&D Article: Games, Gamification and the Quest for Interactive Learning

Fall 2012 Training Industry Quarterly Article 5 Gaming Elements for elearning

Chapter One of the Gamification of Learning and Instruction

If you are interested in an expanded version of the information from above, see the book below.

Additional Related Books:

Click here to visit a catalog of Resources on this subject.

Posted in: ASTD Conference, Avatar, Design, Games

Leave a Comment (0) ↓
Karl Kapp
  • About
  • Contact